Explaining sad people's memory advantage for faces

Authors: Hills, P.J., Marquardt, Z., Young, I. and Goodenough, I.

Journal: Frontiers in Psychology

Volume: 8

Issue: FEB

eISSN: 1664-1078

DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00207

Abstract:

Sad people recognize faces more accurately than happy people (Hills et al., 2011). We devised four hypotheses for this finding that are tested between in the current study. The four hypotheses are: (1) sad people engage in more expert processing associated with face processing; (2) sad people are motivated to be more accurate than happy people in an attempt to repair their mood; (3) sad people have a defocused attentional strategy that allows more information about a face to be encoded; and (4) sad people scan more of the face than happy people leading to more facial features to be encoded. In Experiment 1, we found that dysphoria (sad mood often associated with depression) was not correlated with the face-inversion effect (a measure of expert processing) nor with response times but was correlated with defocused attention and recognition accuracy. Experiment 2 established that dysphoric participants detected changes made to more facial features than happy participants. In Experiment 3, using eye-tracking we found that sad-induced participants sampled more of the face whilst avoiding the eyes. Experiment 4 showed that sad-induced people demonstrated a smaller own-ethnicity bias. These results indicate that sad people show different attentional allocation to faces than happy and neutral people.

https://eprints.bournemouth.ac.uk/27934/

Source: Scopus

Explaining Sad People's Memory Advantage for Faces.

Authors: Hills, P.J., Marquardt, Z., Young, I. and Goodenough, I.

Journal: Front Psychol

Volume: 8

Pages: 207

ISSN: 1664-1078

DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00207

Abstract:

Sad people recognize faces more accurately than happy people (Hills et al., 2011). We devised four hypotheses for this finding that are tested between in the current study. The four hypotheses are: (1) sad people engage in more expert processing associated with face processing; (2) sad people are motivated to be more accurate than happy people in an attempt to repair their mood; (3) sad people have a defocused attentional strategy that allows more information about a face to be encoded; and (4) sad people scan more of the face than happy people leading to more facial features to be encoded. In Experiment 1, we found that dysphoria (sad mood often associated with depression) was not correlated with the face-inversion effect (a measure of expert processing) nor with response times but was correlated with defocused attention and recognition accuracy. Experiment 2 established that dysphoric participants detected changes made to more facial features than happy participants. In Experiment 3, using eye-tracking we found that sad-induced participants sampled more of the face whilst avoiding the eyes. Experiment 4 showed that sad-induced people demonstrated a smaller own-ethnicity bias. These results indicate that sad people show different attentional allocation to faces than happy and neutral people.

https://eprints.bournemouth.ac.uk/27934/

Source: PubMed

Explaining Sad People's Memory Advantage for Faces

Authors: Hills, P.J., Marquardt, Z., Young, I. and Goodenough, I.

Journal: FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY

Volume: 8

ISSN: 1664-1078

DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00207

https://eprints.bournemouth.ac.uk/27934/

Source: Web of Science (Lite)

Explaining Sad People's Memory Advantage for Faces.

Authors: Hills, P.J., Marquardt, Z., Young, I. and Goodenough, I.

Journal: Frontiers in psychology

Volume: 8

Pages: 207

eISSN: 1664-1078

ISSN: 1664-1078

DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00207

Abstract:

Sad people recognize faces more accurately than happy people (Hills et al., 2011). We devised four hypotheses for this finding that are tested between in the current study. The four hypotheses are: (1) sad people engage in more expert processing associated with face processing; (2) sad people are motivated to be more accurate than happy people in an attempt to repair their mood; (3) sad people have a defocused attentional strategy that allows more information about a face to be encoded; and (4) sad people scan more of the face than happy people leading to more facial features to be encoded. In Experiment 1, we found that dysphoria (sad mood often associated with depression) was not correlated with the face-inversion effect (a measure of expert processing) nor with response times but was correlated with defocused attention and recognition accuracy. Experiment 2 established that dysphoric participants detected changes made to more facial features than happy participants. In Experiment 3, using eye-tracking we found that sad-induced participants sampled more of the face whilst avoiding the eyes. Experiment 4 showed that sad-induced people demonstrated a smaller own-ethnicity bias. These results indicate that sad people show different attentional allocation to faces than happy and neutral people.

https://eprints.bournemouth.ac.uk/27934/

Source: Europe PubMed Central

Explaining Sad People's Memory Advantage for Faces.

Authors: Hills, P.J., Marquardt, Z., Young, I. and Goodenough, I.

Journal: Frontiers in Psychology

Volume: 8

Pages: 207

ISSN: 1664-1078

Abstract:

Sad people recognize faces more accurately than happy people (Hills et al., 2011). We devised four hypotheses for this finding that are tested between in the current study. The four hypotheses are: (1) sad people engage in more expert processing associated with face processing; (2) sad people are motivated to be more accurate than happy people in an attempt to repair their mood; (3) sad people have a defocused attentional strategy that allows more information about a face to be encoded; and (4) sad people scan more of the face than happy people leading to more facial features to be encoded. In Experiment 1, we found that dysphoria (sad mood often associated with depression) was not correlated with the face-inversion effect (a measure of expert processing) nor with response times but was correlated with defocused attention and recognition accuracy. Experiment 2 established that dysphoric participants detected changes made to more facial features than happy participants. In Experiment 3, using eye-tracking we found that sad-induced participants sampled more of the face whilst avoiding the eyes. Experiment 4 showed that sad-induced people demonstrated a smaller own-ethnicity bias. These results indicate that sad people show different attentional allocation to faces than happy and neutral people.

https://eprints.bournemouth.ac.uk/27934/

Source: BURO EPrints